Chapter 44: Internal order of tense, aspect, and mood markers

Feature information for this chapter can be found in feature 44.

1. Introduction

In this feature we ask about the order of tense, aspect, and mood markers (TAM markers) with respect to each other. To qualify for this feature, it is important that the three markers be adjacent to each other. A context which favours the combination of the TAM markers is counterfactual clauses, where the tense marker usually corresponds to a past marker, the aspect marker to an imperfective marker, and the mood marker to a future marker (used modally). An example is Creolese, where the order of the three markers is tense-aspect-mood (TAM):

(1)
Dem
3pl
bin
tns
a
asp
go
mood
raab
rob
mi.
1sg
They would have robbed me.

Note that we consider markers to be adjacent even if lexical items such as adverbs may intervene between them (see Chapter 45 and Chapter 46 on the tightness of the link between the past and the progressive marker with the verb); what matters is that the markers are located on the same side of the verbal complex.

There are many APiCS languages to which this feature does not apply, either because they do not possess three markers, or, if they have three markers, they do not allow them to combine or to be adjacent to each other.

As in other chapters, we use “aspect” in a restricted sense, applying it only to the opposition between perfective and imperfective aspect.

This feature has played an important role in creole studies since it was one of the features of Bickerton’s language bioprogram hypothesis. According to Bickerton, creole languages display the order TMA when the three markers combine (see below our value 1), where the category ‘mood’ is defined as ‘irrealis’, referring to markers which express future, counterfactual, conditional, and similar functions (Bickerton 1980: 6). Note, however, that Bickerton was not the first linguist to make this claim. To our knowledge the first linguist was Voorhoeve (1957: 384).

2. The values

We distinguish the following four values:

exclshrdall
Tense-Mood-Aspect11112
Tense-Aspect-Mood202
Mood-Tense-Aspect314
The feature does not apply56056
Representation:73

This box shows that the possibility of using tense, aspect, and mood markers adjacent to each other is restricted to a relatively small subset of APiCS languages (about 25%); furthermore, the box also shows that the logically possible combinations MAT, ATM, and AMT are not attested in our languages.

Value 1 (Tense-Mood-Aspect) occurs in six English-based and in six French-based languages.

(2)
If
if
ren
rain
neg
bin
pst
de
prog
kam,
come
wi
1pl
bin
pst
mood
de
prog
ple
play
bɔl.
ball
If it hadn't been raining, we would have been playing soccer.
(3)
Li
3sg
ti
pst
ava
mood
pe
asp
aprann
learn
si
if
li
3sg
ti
pst
anvi
want
pas
pass
so
poss.3sg
lexame.
exam
She would have been studying if she had wanted to pass her exams.

Value 2 (Tense-Aspect-Mood) is found in Creolese (see example 1) and in Kinubi:

(4)
kan
pst
bi
fut
gi
prog
ruwa
go
would have been going

Value 3 (Mood-Tense-Aspect) is found in Santome, in Principense, and in Reunion Creole; note, however, that in Reunion Creole the confidence value is given as ‘uncertain’.

(5)
Xi
if
ê
3sg
mood
ta
pst
ka
ipfv
kume;
eat
ami
1sg
tudaxi
also
ka
ipfv
kume.
eat
If he had been eating, I also would have been eating.
(6)
Xi
if
non
1pl
ka
mood
tava
pst
sa
prog
xivi
work
wosê,
now
non
1pl
ka
mood
tava
pst
have
dyô.
money
If we were working now, we would have money.

In some languages, for example Guyanais, Kinubi, or Principense, the mood marker occurring in combination with the tense and the aspect marker also functions as a future marker, but in other languages like Nengee or Krio this is not the case. In Nengee, for instance, the future marker is o and the counterfactual marker is sa, and in Krio the markers are go and . However, it looks as if there has been some diachronic change from future and counterfactual marker to only counterfactual marker, at least in Sranan (which is closely related to Nengee). In Early Sranan, sa is the only marker attested as a future marker, and it also occurs in counterfactual and similar contexts. The following example illustrates value 1 (TMA):

(7)
Joe
2sg
ben
pst
sa
fut
dee
asp
leesie?
be.lazy
Would you be lazy?

In modern Sranan, sa only occurs in counterfactual and similar contexts, and the future marker is go. This suggests that the grammaticalization of the verb go as a future marker and the specialization of sa as a (non-future) mood marker is a later development.

Modern Sranan is the only APiCS language that allows different orders. Example (8) illustrates value 1 (TMA) and example (9) value 3 (MTA):

(8)
Efu
if
John
John
ben
pst
de
cop
dya,
here
a
3sg
ben
pst
sa
mood
e
prog
sribi
sleep
now.
now
If John were here, he would be sleeping now.
(9)
Nownow
now
de
there
Tanti
Aunt
M.
M.
sa
mood
ben
pst
e
ipfv
ferteri
tell
stori
story
efu
if
a
3sg
ben
pst
de
cop
na
loc
libi
life
ete.
yet
Aunt M. would have been telling stories right now if she were still alive.

Note that according to Winford & Plag (2013), example (9) was only accepted by one older informant.

As noted above, value 4 (the feature does not apply) concerns (i) languages which do not have all three markers, (ii) languages which do not allow them to combine, or (iii) languages which do not allow them to be adjacent to each other. Among this last group we find for example Papiamentu, where the future marker lo has partly retained the pre-subject position of its Portuguese adverbial etymon (logo ‘right away’) and where the past marker taba has fused with the originally progressive marker ta (note that taba is not used without ta except for the verbs ta ‘be’ and tin ‘have’, as in the following example):

(10)
Papiamentu (Maurer own knowledge)
Lo
fut
e
3sg
tabata
pst.prog
traha
work
si
if
e
3sg
tabatin
pst.have
tempu.
time
He would be working if he had time.

In several languages, some markers are preverbal and others postverbal (see also the examples in Chapter 43 on the position of TAM markers in relation to the verb):

(11)
N'
1sg
na
prog
ta
fut
bay
go
ba
pst
skola
school
ma
but
N'
1sg
sta
cop
ba
pst
dwenti.
sick
I would have gone to school, but I was sick.
(12)
Im-in
3sg-pst
oldei
always
nes-im-bat
nurse-tr-prog
tu
too
jet
dem
faiya.
fire
It used to look after the fire too.
(13)
Yena
3sg
zo
fut
gate
ant
hamb-ile.
go-pst
He will have gone.

3. Distribution

Among the APiCS languages, the possibility of combining tense, aspect, and mood markers adjacent to each other occurs only in creole languages. This possibility is also restricted areally: it occurs in the Atlantic, the Indian Ocean, and in East Africa. In the Atlantic, it occurs in Portuguese-based, in English-based, and in French-based creoles; in the Indian Ocean, it occurs in the three French-based creoles. Note that the Portuguese-based creoles exhibiting this feature are restricted to the Gulf of Guinea; in East Africa, this feature occurs in Kinubi, which is Arabic-based.

The results of this chapter cast some doubts on Bickerton’s claim that the TMA pattern is universal. First, the possibility of combining the tense, the aspect, and the mood marker only occurs in seventeen languages; second, these seventeen languages are areally restricted, and third, six of the seventeen languages which do allow the combination of the three markers do not have TMA, but TAM or MTA.